- Journal List
- Scientific Reports
- PMC10151354
As a library, NLM provides access to scientific literature. Inclusion in an NLM database does not imply endorsement of, or agreement with, the contents by NLM or the National Institutes of Health.
Learn more: PMC Disclaimer | PMC Copyright Notice
Sci Rep. 2023; 13: 7091.
Published online 2023 May 1. doi:10.1038/s41598-023-33996-1
PMCID: PMC10151354
PMID: 37127723
Weixi Kang,1 Francois Steffens,2 Sònia Pineda,3 Kaya Widuch,4 and Antonio Malvaso5,6
Author information Article notes Copyright and License information PMC Disclaimer
Associated Data
- Data Availability Statement
Abstract
Individuals are different in a relatively constant pattern of thoughts, feeling, and behaviors, which are called personality traits. Mental health is a condition of well-being in which people may reach their full potential and deal effectively with stress, work efficiently, and contribute to their communities. Indeed, the link between personality and mental health as indicated by the 12-item version of the general health questionnaires (GHQ-12) has been well-established according to evidence found by decades of research. However, the GHQ-12 comprises many questions asking about different dimensions of mental health. It is unclear how personality traits relate to these dimensions of mental health. In this paper, we try to address this question. We analyzed data from 12,007 participants from the British Household Panel Study (BHPS) using a confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) and generalized linear models. We replicated the factor structure of GHQ-12 labeled as GHQ-12A (social dysfunction & anhedonia; 6 items), GHQ-12B (depression & anxiety; 4 items), and GHQ-12C (loss of confidence; 2 items). Moreover, Neuroticism was positively related to all dimensions of mental health issues, Extraversion was negatively related to GHQ-12A (social dysfunction & anhedonia) and GHQ-12B (depression & anxiety), Agreeableness and Conscientiousness were negatively related to GHQ-12A (social dysfunction & anhedonia) and GHQ-12C (loss of confidence), and Openness was negatively related to GHQ-12B (depression & anxiety). These results contribute to theories including the predisposition/vulnerability model, complication/scar model, pathoplasty/exacerbation model, and the spectrum model, which propose that personality traits are linked to mental health and explained possible reasons. Psychologists may use results from this study to identify individuals who may be at high risk of developing various non-psychiatric mental health issues and intervene to avoid negative outcomes.
Subject terms: Psychology, Human behaviour
Introduction
Individuals distinct in a relatively constant pattern of thoughts, feelings, and behaviors, and these differences can be captured by personality traits. Personality traits have been categorized as "essential psychological constructs" are because they have a significant impact on important life aspects of health-related behaviors e.g.,1,2, and the likelihood of psychopathology e.g.,3,4, crime e.g.,5, work experiences e.g.,6,7, academic achievement e.g.,8, romantic relationships e.g.,9,10 and parent–child interaction11. Nevertheless, it is unusual for social scientists to find a single domain of interests in which no evidence supporting the importance of personality traits has been presented. Personality psychologists generally believe that there are five major dimensions that can be used to categorize a wide range of possible personality traits. These dimensions are referred to as the Big Five, and they include Extraversion, Neuroticism, Agreeableness, Conscientiousness, and Openness to experience12. Extraversion refers to differences among individuals in terms of their friendliness, sociability, level of activity, and experience of positive emotions. Agreeableness refers to differences among individuals in terms of their altruistic behavior, trust, warmth, and friendliness. Conscientiousness refers to differences among individuals in terms of their ability to control their impulses, focus on tasks, and follow rules. Neuroticism refers to differences among individuals in terms of their susceptibility to negative emotions such as anxiety, anger, and sadness. Finally, Openness to experience refers to differences among individuals in terms of their creativity, innovation, and willingness to accept new ideas13. The widespread acceptance of the Big Five framework provides a systematic way to define personality differences at the most fundamental levels. This has helped researchers accumulate knowledge about how personality traits are related to various life outcomes.
Psychological health is a significant aspect in total happiness. Mental health, according to the World Health Organization14, is "a condition of well-being in which each person fulfills his or her own potential, can cope with typical stressors of life, can work successfully and fruitfully, and can contribute to her or his community". Traditionally, healthcare providers have been able to accurately assess an individual's well-being by looking at their substance misuse, anxiety, distress, and depression15. As a result, mental health is described as a state of complete physical, mental, and social well-being rather than the absence of psychiatric diseases14. The general health questionnaire (GHQ) is a widely used self-reported questionnaire that has been developed by Goldberg16. The GHQ is known for being a reliable indicator of mental health16–20. It has been used extensively in different settings, including cross-cultural settings17,18, primary health care, and outpatient settings to screen for psychological diseases16,19,20. Furthermore, the GHQ has been utilized in demographic research and health assessment surveys21.
Recently, many studies began to examine the factor structure of the 12-item version of the GHQ (GHQ-12), although the GHQ-12 was originally created as a unidimensional scale with a few studies use the one-factor latent structure22,23. Instead of using a single factor model, other models with 2 or 3 factors have been found to be more suitable. Among these, the 3-factor model has received more empirical support based on research studies24–28. Specifically, the three components in the model include the GHQ-12A (social dysfunction and anhedonia; 6 items), GHQ-12B (depression and anxiety; 4 items), and GHQ-12C (loss of confidence; 2 items).
Personality traits has been long linked to psychopathology, as shown by several models including the predisposition/vulnerability model, complication/scar model, pathoplasty/exacerbation model, and the spectrum model29–32. Other than psychopathology, it has been proposed personality as a strong predictor of general psychological health33–35, which comprises positive mental health/wellbeing36–38. Healthy personality development contributes to many areas of well-being and there is a necessity to include personality’s contributions to well-being into current treatments to mental health39–41. The five-factor model of personality (FFM) suggests that Neuroticism and Extraversion are the personality traits that are most strongly associated with mental health42–47. People who score high on the Neuroticism trait tend to experience negative emotions, respond poorly to stress, and may struggle with impulsivity and psychological distress48–53. On the other hand, those who score high on Extraversion tend to enjoy social interactions, feel positive emotions more easily, and have better mental health outcomes54–67. However, people who score high on Agreeableness may have worse mental health outcomes, while those who score high on Openness and Conscientiousness may have better outcomes54,68–70.
Although many studies have investigated how personality could predict mental health, few studies have investigated how they may relate to dimensions of mental health as mental health is never a unitary concept. To understand how personality traits are associated with dimensions of mental health, we first produce three underlying factors of GHQ-12 and investigate how personality traits are related to dimensions of mental health. We hypothesize that Neuroticism and Extraversion have the strongest positive associations with dimensions of mental health issues whereas other associations between personality traits and dimensions of mental health may vary across dimensions of mental health. Specifically, Agreeableness should be negatively associated with mental health whereas Openness and Conscientiousness are expected to be negatively related to various mental health issues.
Methods
Data
We used data from the British Household Panel Study (BHPS)71, which is an ongoing longitudinal survey of representative samples of individual households in the UK since 1991. Participants were interviewed in person once a year. The data were collected from September, 2005 to May, 2006 with ethical guidelines following ethical approval by the University of Essex Ethics Committee. This particular dataset was used because it is the only wave that contains personality measures. Informed consent has been obtained from all participants.
Predictors
BHPS respondents completed an abbreviated 15-item version of the Big Five Inventory13,72–74 using a 7- point scale ranging from 1 (‘Does not apply to me’) to 7 (‘Applies perfectly to me’). Each dimension of the Big Five consisted of 3 items. Questions that were used to assess the Big Five personality traits can be found in TableTable1.1. Questions optrt5a1, optrt5c2, optrt5e3, and optrt5n3 were reverse coded as these questions were asked in the opposite direction of the corresponding trait. Mean scores were used for each personality traits. The internal consistency analyses revealed the following results: Extraversion (alpha = 0.55), Neuroticism (alpha = 0.68), Conscientiousness (alpha = 0.52), Agreeableness (alpha = 0.53), and Openness (alpha = 0.67). Although these results do not indicate high internal consistency across all five scales, this is not an unusual observation for abbreviated inventories e.g.,75. Nevertheless, Donnellan & Lucas72 confirmed the 3-item shortened scales were strongly correlated with the full versions of the Big Five Inventory and therefore can be considered as an effective replacement.
Table 1
The 15-item version of the BFI, including questions regarding agreeableness, conscientiousness, extraversion, neuroticism, and openness. each dimension of personality consists of three questions.
BFI | ||
---|---|---|
Agreeableness | optrt5a1 | I see myself as someone who is sometimes rude to others |
optrt5a2 | I see myself as someone who has a forgiving nature | |
optrt5a3 | I see myself as someone who is considerate and kind to almost everyone | |
Conscientiousness | optrt5c1 | I see myself as someone who does a thorough job |
optrt5c2 | I see myself as someone who tends to be lazy | |
optrt5c3 | I see myself as someone who does things efficiently | |
Extraversion | optrt5e1 | I see myself as someone who is talkative |
optrt5e2 | I see myself as someone who is outgoing, sociable | |
optrt5e3 | I see myself as someone who is reserved | |
Neuroticism | optrt5n1 | I see myself as someone who worries a lot |
optrt5n2 | I see myself as someone who gets nervously easily | |
optrt5n3 | I see myself as someone who is relaxed, handles stress well | |
Openness | optrt5o1 | I see myself as someone who is original, comes up with new ideas |
optrt5o2 | I see myself as someone who values artistic, aesthetic experiences | |
optrt5o3 | I see myself as someone who has an active imagination |
Predicted variables
The GHQ-12 is simple to administer and can be completed by a single participant in less than 10min22. The original GHQ consisted of 60 items and has a number of different versions such as the GHQ-12, GHQ-20, GHQ-28 and GHQ-30. Given its ease of use, the GHQ-12 is one of the most commonly used versions among those listed23,24. The GHQ-12 is a self-reported 12-item questionnaire with four indexes for each item. The Likert scoring approach (0–1-2–3) and the bi-modal (0–0-1–1) scoring system are two of the most widely used scoring systems22. Banks et al.25 have shown the effectiveness of utilizing the GHQ-12 to compare degrees of psychiatric impairment within and between groups. Several studies have validated the psychometric features of this questionnaire26,27–30. The GHQ-12 has been demonstrated to have strong specificity, reliability, and reasonably high sensitivity31,32. Thus, since Goldberg’s development of the GHQ, it has been used in a variety of countries and cultures, and it has been translated into 38 languages33–37. BHPS respondents completed questions asking their age, sex, present legal marital status, highest educational qualification, political party supported, employment status, and questions from the 12-item GHQ (Table(Table2),2), which used a 7- point scale22 ranging from 1 (‘Better than usual’) to 7 (‘Much less than usual’). The internal consistency of the GHQ-12 is 0.90 (alpha = 0.90).
Table 2
The GHQ-12 consisting of 12 self-reported questions that assess an individual’s general mental health.
GHQ-12 | ||
---|---|---|
oghqa | Have you recently | Been able to concentrate on whatever you're doing? |
oghqb | Have you recently | Lost much sleep over worry? |
oghqc | Have you recently | Felt that you were playing a useful part in things? |
oghqd | Have you recently | felt capable of making decisions about things? |
oghqe | Have you recently | Felt constantly under strain? |
oghqf | Have you recently | Felt you couldn't overcome your difficulties? |
oghqg | Have you recently | Been able to enjoy your normal day-to- day activities? |
oghqh | Have you recently | Been able to face up to problems? |
oghqi | Have you recently | Been feeling unhappy or depressed? |
oghqj | Have you recently | Been losing confidence in yourself? |
oghqk | Have you recently | Been thinking of yourself as a worthless person? |
oghql | Have you recently | Been feeling reasonably happy, all things considered? |
Analysis
There was data from 15, 617 participants from SHPS Wave 15 in total. Participants who had any missing data field and who were older than 99 or younger than 16 were removed from further analysis because of extremely low numbers of participants in these groups. Thus, a total of 12, 007 data points from participants remained.
Factor model
Answers from GHQ 12 were taken into a confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) with a specified number of factors 3 in MATLAB 2018a. The three-factor scores for each respondent were computed as the mean of the responses to the items provided by the respondent. Specifically, the three factors were labeled as GHQ-12A (social dysfunction & anhedonia; 6 items), GHQ-12B (depression & anxiety; 4 items), and GHQ-12C (loss of confidence; 2 items).
Linear models
We examined how Big Five personality traits including Neuroticism, Openness, Agreeableness, Conscientiousness, and Extraversion could predict dimensions of mental health by performing three multiple regressions using demographics and Big Five personality traits including Neuroticism, Openness, Agreeableness, Conscientiousness, and Extraversion and demographics as predictors and GHQ-12A (social dysfunction & anhedonia), GHQ-12B (depression & anxiety), and GHQ-12C (loss of confidence) as predicted variables.
Results
Descriptive statistics can be found in TableTable3.3. The CFA yielded three interpretable factors including GHQ-12A (social dysfunction & anhedonia; 6 items), GHQ-12B (depression & anxiety; 4 items), and GHQ-12C (loss of confidence; 2 items). The loadings of these items can be found in TableTable4.4. All the items of the GHQ-12 loaded on the factors they were expected to24–28.
Table 3
Descriptive statistics of variables of interest.
Variable name | Value | Count (n) | Percent (%) |
---|---|---|---|
Sex | Male | 7120 | 45.6 |
Female | 8497 | 54.4 | |
Present legal marital status | Married | 8115 | 51.96 |
Separated | 327 | 2.09 | |
Divorced | 1248 | 7.99 | |
Widowed | 1192 | 7.63 | |
Never married | 4,727 | 30.27 | |
Highest educational qualification Political party supported | Higher degree | 425 | 2.7 |
First degree | 1,597 | 10.2 | |
Teaching QF | 339 | 2.2 | |
Other higher QF | 3432 | 22.0 | |
Nursing QF | 161 | 1.0 | |
GCE A levels | 1811 | 11.6 | |
GCE O levels or equi | 2517 | 16.1 | |
Commercial QF, No O | 331 | 2.1 | |
CSE Grade 2–5, Scot G | 421 | 2.7 | |
Apprenticeship | 257 | 1.6 | |
Other QF | 102 | 0.7 | |
No QF | 2787 | 17.8 | |
Still at school No Q | 138 | 0.9 | |
Conservative | 2372 | 15.2 | |
Labor | 3964 | 25.4 | |
Lib Dem/LiB/SDP | 1606 | 10.3 | |
Scot Nat | 396 | 2.5 | |
Plaid Cymru | 206 | 1.3 | |
Green Party | 179 | 1.1 | |
Other party | 142 | 0.9 | |
Other answer | 54 | 0.3 | |
None | 2,213 | 14.2 | |
Cannot vote | 287 | 1.8 | |
Ulster unionist | 480 | 3.1 | |
SDLP | 457 | 2.9 | |
Alliance party | 119 | 0.8 | |
Democratice unionist | 504 | 3.2 | |
Sinn fein | 233 | 1.5 | |
Other party | 42 | 0.3 | |
Employment status | Self employed | 1030 | 6.60 |
In paid employ | 7318 | 46.86 | |
Unemployed | 486 | 3.11 | |
Retired | 3,074 | 19.68 | |
Maternity leave | 94 | 0.60 | |
Family care | 953 | 6.10 | |
FT studt, school | 877 | 5.62 | |
LT sick, disabled | 656 | 4.20 | |
Govt trng scheme | 24 | 0.15 | |
Something else | 106 | 0.68 |
Table 4
The factor loadings for the three-factor structure of the GHQ-12.
GHQ-12 items | GHQ-12A (social dysfunction & anhedonia; 6 items) | GHQ-12B (depression & anxiety; 4 items) | GHQ-12C (loss of confidence; 2 items) |
---|---|---|---|
Concentration | 0.55 | 0.25 | − 0.11 |
Loss of sleep | − 0.03 | 0.73 | 0.00 |
Playing a useful Role | 0.73 | − 0.21 | 0.12 |
Constantly under Strain | 0.83 | − 0.20 | 0.05 |
Problem overcoming difficulties | − 0.10 | 0.88 | − 0.05 |
Unhappy or Depressed | 0.04 | 0.59 | 0.18 |
Losing Confidence | 0.56 | 0.33 | − 0.16 |
Believe worthless | 0.69 | 0.03 | 0.01 |
General Happiness | 0.01 | 0.63 | 0.24 |
Capable of making decisions | 0.02 | 0.22 | 0.68 |
Ability to face Problems | 0.09 | 0.03 | 0.75 |
Enjoy day− to− day activities | 0.50 | 0.21 | 0.06 |
The heaviest loading value for each questionis in bold.
Demographics and personality traits explained 21.3% (adjusted R2 = 0.213) variances of GHQ-12B (depression & anxiety). Specifically, Neuroticism (β = 0.34; t = 52.05, p < 0.001; 95% C.I. [0.33, 0.35]), Extraversion (β = 0.03; t = 3.76, p < 0.001; 95% C.I. [0.01, 0.04]) and Openness (β =− 0.02; t = 2.23, p = 0.03; 95% C.I. [− 0.04, 0.00]) was positively related to GHQ-12B (depression & anxiety) after controlling for demographics (Table(Table55).
Table 5
Estimates (β) of demographics and personality predictors for GHQ-12B (depression & anxiety).
Variables | β | SE | tStat | p Value | 95% C.I |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Intercept | − 1.34 | 0.08 | 16.60 | < 0.001 | [− 1.50, –1.18] |
Age | 0.00 | 0.00 | 0.36 | 0.72 | [0.00, 0.00] |
Sex | 0.05 | 0.02 | 2.70 | 0.01 | [0.01, 0.08] |
Present legal marital status | − 0.02 | 0.01 | 3.24 | 0.001 | [− 0.03, − 0.01] |
Highest educational qualification | 0.00 | 0.00 | 1.26 | 0.21 | [0.00, 0.01] |
Political party supported | 0.00 | 0.00 | 0.10 | 0.92 | [0.00, 0.00] |
Annual income (1.9.2004–1.9.2005) | 0.00 | 0.00 | 3.15 | 0.002 | [0.00, 0.00] |
Employment status | 0.01 | 0.00 | 2.20 | 0.03 | [0.00, 0.02] |
Neuroticism | 0.34 | 0.01 | 52.05 | < 0.001 | [0.33, 0.35] |
Openness | − 0.02 | 0.01 | 2.23 | 0.03 | [− 0.04, 0.00] |
Agreeableness | 0.01 | 0.01 | 0.99 | 0.32 | [− 0.01, 0.02] |
Conscientiousness | − 0.01 | 0.01 | 1.45 | 0.15 | [− 0.03, 0.00] |
Extraversion | 0.03 | 0.01 | 3.76 | < 0.001 | [0.01, 0.04] |
Demographics and personality traits explained 10.7% (adjusted R2 = 0.107) variances of GHQ-12A (social dysfunction & anhedonia). Specifically, Neuroticism (β = 0.14; t = 19.95, p < 0.001; 95% C.I. [0.13, 0.15]) was positively related to GHQ-12A (social dysfunction & anhedonia) whereas Extraversion (β =− 0.07; t =− 9.04, p < 0.001; 95% C.I. [− 0.09, –0.06]) Conscientiousness (β = − 0.04; t =− 3.98, p < 0.001; 95% C.I. [− 0.05, –0.02]) and Agreeableness (β = − 0.02; t = − 2.46, p = 0.01; 95% C.I. [− 0.04, 0.00]) were negatively related to GHQ-12A (social dysfunction & anhedonia) after controlling for demographics (Table(Table66).
Table 6
Estimates (β) of demographics and personality predictors for GHQ-12A (social dysfunction & anhedonia).
Variables | β | SE | tStat | p Value | 95% C.I |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Intercept | − 0.59 | 0.09 | − 6.86 | < 0.001 | [− 0.76, –0.42] |
Age | 0.01 | 0.00 | 19.14 | < 0.001 | [0.01, 0.01] |
Sex | − 0.03 | 0.02 | − 1.46 | 0.14 | [− 0.06, 0.01] |
Present legal marital status | 0.00 | 0.01 | − 0.58 | 0.56 | [− 0.01, 0.01] |
Highest educational qualification | 0.09 | 0.00 | 2.93 | 0.003 | [0.00, 0.01] |
Political party supported | 0.00 | 0.00 | 1.19 | 0.23 | [0.00, 0.01] |
Annual income (1.9.2004–1.9.2005) | 0.00 | 0.00 | 1.65 | 0.10 | [0.00, 0.00] |
Employment status | 0.03 | 0.00 | 6.52 | < 0.001 | [0.02, 0.04] |
Neuroticism | 0.14 | 0.01 | 19.95 | < 0.001 | [0.13, 0.15] |
Openness | 0.01 | 0.01 | 0.87 | 0.38 | [− 0.01, 0.03] |
Agreeableness | − 0.02 | 0.01 | − 2.46 | 0.01 | [− 0.04, 0.00] |
Conscientiousness | − 0.04 | 0.01 | − 3.98 | < 0.001 | [− 0.05, –0.02] |
Extraversion | − 0.07 | 0.01 | − 9.04 | < 0.001 | [− 0.09, –0.06] |
Demographics and personality traits explained 17.9% (adjusted R2 = 0.179) variances of GHQ-12C (loss of confidence). Specifically, Neuroticism (β = 0.26; t = 38.23, p < 0.001; 95% C.I. [0.24, 0.27]) was positively related to GHQ-12C (loss of confidence) and Conscientiousness (β = − 0.10; t = 11.73, p < 0.001; 95% C.I. [− 0.12, –0.08]), and Agreeableness (β = − 0.06; t = 7.25, p < 0.001; 95% C.I. [− 0.07, –0.04]) were negatively related to GHQ-12C (loss of confidence) after controlling for demographics (Table(Table77)
Table 7
Estimates (β) of demographics and personality predictors for GHQ-12C (loss of confidence).
Variables | β | SE | tStat | p Value | 95% C.I |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Intercept | − 0.41 | 0.08 | 5.00 | < 0.001 | [− 0.57, –0.25] |
Age | 0.00 | 0.00 | 4.39 | < 0.001 | [0.00, 0.00] |
Sex | 0.02 | 0.02 | 1.17 | 0.24 | [− 0.01, 0.06] |
Present legal marital status | 0.01 | 0.01 | 2.86 | 0.004 | [0.01, 0.02] |
Highest educational qualification | 0.01 | 0.00 | 3.82 | < 0.001 | [0.01, 0.02] |
Political party supported | 0.00 | 0.00 | 1.95 | 0.05 | [0.00, 0.01] |
Annual income | 0.00 | 0.00 | 1.83 | 0.07 | [0.00, 0.00] |
Employment status | 0.03 | 0.01 | 6.50 | < 0.001 | [0.02, 0.04] |
Neuroticism | 0.26 | 0.01 | 38.23 | < 0.001 | [0.24, 0.27] |
Openness | − 0.01 | 0.01 | 1.42 | 0.16 | [− 0.03, 0.01] |
Agreeableness | − 0.06 | 0.01 | 7.25 | < 0.001 | [− 0.07, –0.04] |
Conscientiousness | − 0.10 | 0.01 | 11.73 | < 0.001 | [− 0.12, –0.08] |
Extraversion | − 0.01 | 0.01 | 0.66 | 0.51 | [− 0.02, 0.01] |
Discussion
Taken together, the aim of the current study was to investigate how Big Five personality traits are associated with dimensions of mental health as measured by GHQ-12. We used a CFA along with three linear models to replicate the findings of previous studies regarding the three factors present within the GHQ-12 questionnaire. These factors include GHQ-12A (social dysfunction & anhedonia), GHQ-12B (depression & anxiety), and GHQ-12C (loss of confidence). The factor loadings in the current study were found to be consistent with those of previous studies24–28, with heavy loadings on corresponding items. This study provided novel findings regarding how personality traits may relate to dimensions of mental health as mental health is never a unitary concept.
We also found that Neuroticism is positively related to all components of mental health including GHQ-12A (social dysfunction & anhedonia), GHQ-12B (depression & anxiety), and GHQ-12C (loss of confidence) after controlling for demographics. People who are neurotic60 have more negative effects (e.g., GHQ-12A (social dysfunction & anhedonia), GHQ-12B (depression & anxiety), and GHQ-12C (loss of confidence)). These results are harmonious with previous studies that found Neuroticism is related to low subjective well-being62, depressive symptoms, anxiety, mood, and substance abuse disorders23,47–49. These results may be explained by the possibility that individuals who are through a depressive or anxiety episode may endorse more of these overlapping Neuroticism items during or following the event. Yet, the link between Neuroticism and outcomes related to public health is more than just a result of overlapping criteria. Several longitudinal research have examined the relationship between the notion of Neuroticism and depressive scores while controlling for shared items and contemporaneous depressed moods76–78. Additionally, as a summary of previous research indicates, Neuroticism is strongly positively associated with a variety of mental disorders, including schizophrenia, eating disorders, somatoform disorders, and substance use disorders, as well as physical health issues that are not specifically identified by symptoms that coincide with Neuroticism items. Similarly, Openness was also positively related to GHQ-12B (depression & anxiety) according to previous studies. For instance, it has been suggested that depressed participants showed significantly higher Opennessscores than participants without depression79. However, a longitudinal study found that change in Openness scores did not relate with the occurrence of or the recovery from any depressive or anxiety disorder80. The reasons that our results differ can be explained by the fact that we used different instruments for personality and mental health assessments.
The results showed that Agreeableness is a significant positive predictor of two dimensions of mental health including GHQ-12A (social dysfunction & anhedonia) and GHQ-12C (loss of confidence). While there is little evidence to suggest that Agreeableness is related to social dysfunction and anhedonia, Yu et al. (2020) found a positive relationship between Agreeableness and overall social well-being81. Another study found a weak but significant relationship between social anxiety and Agreeableness82. Our findings seem to be consistent with these previous studies. Additionally, our study found that Agreeableness is positively related to confidence in older adults83, which is in line with the notion that Agreeableness and overconfidence are positively associated84. Finally, our findings also support previous research suggesting that Agreeableness is unaffected by depression and anxiety80.
Moreover, we found that Conscientiousness is predictive of GHQ-12A (social dysfunction & anhedonia) and GHQ-12C (loss of confidence). Previous research studied the relationship between social dysfunction and Conscientiousness85–88. Moreover, it was found that increasingthe likelihood of a conscientious and socially stable population could have significant health benefits, despite existing evidence indicates that the causal relationships between Conscientiousness and social dysfunction are multifaceted and complex85,86,88. The finding that Conscientiousness is negatively associated with loss of confidence seem to be consistent with previous findings89.
Consistent with common beliefs e.g.,90, Extraversion was related to GHQ-12A (social dysfunction & anhedonia) and GHQ-12B (depression & anxiety). Several studies investigated the relationship between Extraversion and social dysfunction87,91,92. According to previous reviews91,93, the negative correlations between Extraversion and depression are primarily due to the aspect of Communal Extraversion, the consensual facet of Liveliness, and NEO Positive Emotions. Changes in Extraversion were also linked to changes in depressive disorder and anxiety disorder status80.
Some limitations of the current study should be considered in evaluating the findings presented in this study. First, a brief self-report 15-item scale wasused to assess personality traits. As reported by different studies94–96, the short BFI measure has already been demonstrated to have good psychometric features, and relatively short questionnaires work reasonably well in personality research97. Furthermore, studies like this are always hampered by common method variance, with self-report serving as both the predictors and predicted variables. It is frequently desirable to have observer’s and behavioral data to supplement self-report data, and the methodology could account for a portion of the shared variance. Moreover, the relationships between personality traits and dimensions of mental health can be bi-directional as there might be some innate common underlying genetic factors given the survey questions in the Big Five and GHQ-12 may overlap to some extent. Finally, the datawas quite old, which may limit its generalizability as society evolves quickly.
In conclusion, we explored the relationship between personality and each dimension of mental health. Our results suggest when looking at the relationship between mental health using personality and the GHQ-12, it is important to consider the finer-grained detail of what the GHQ-12 is asking. Thus, mental health is not a unitary concept but has many dimensionalities, and personality traits are associated with them differently. These results contribute to theories including the predisposition/vulnerability model, complication/scar model, pathoplasty/exacerbation model, and the spectrum model29–32, which propose that personality traits are linked to mental health and explained possible reasons. Psychologists may use results from this study to identify individuals who may be at high risk of developing various non-psychiatric mental health issues and intervene to avoid negative outcomes.
Author contributions
W.K.: conceptualization, methodology, writing–original draft, writing–review & editing, and funding acquisition. F.S.: formal analysis. S.P.: writing–original draft and writing–review & editing. K.W.: writing–original draft and writing–review & editing. A.M.: writing-review & editing.
Data availability
The study materials and data can be accessed at https://www.iser.essex.ac.uk/bhps/documentation/volb/wave15.
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Footnotes
Publisher's note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
References
1. Bogg T, Roberts BW. Conscientiousness and health-related behaviors: A meta-analysis of the leading behavioral contributors to mortality. Psychol. Bull. 2004;130:887–919. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.130.6.887. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
2. Friedman HF, et al. Does childhood personality predict longevity? J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1993;65:176–185. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.65.1.176. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
3. Krueger RF, Caspi A, Moffitt TE, Silva PA, McGee R. Personality traits are differentially linked to mental disorders: A multitrait-multidiagnosis study of an adolescent birth cohort. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 1996;105:299–312. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.105.3.299. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
4. Krueger RF, Caspi A, Moffitt TE. Epidemiological personology: The unifying role of personality in population-based research on problem behaviors. J. Pers. 2000;68:967–998. doi:10.1111/1467-6494.00123. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
5. Miller JD, Lynam D. Structural models of personality and their relation to antisocial behavior: A meta-analytic review. Criminology. 2001;39:765–798. doi:10.1111/j.1745-9125.2001.tb00940.x. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
6. Barrick MR, Mount MK. The big five personality dimensions and job performance: A meta-analysis. Person. Psychol. 1991;44:1–26. doi:10.1111/j.1744-6570.1991.tb00688.x. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
7. Judge TA, Higgins CA, Thoresen CJ, Barrick MR. The big five personality traits, general mental ability, and career success across the life span. Person. Psychol. 1999;52:621–652. doi:10.1111/j.1744-6570.1999.tb00174.x. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
8. Digman JM. Five robust trait dimensions: Development, stability, and utility. J. Pers. 1989;57:195–214. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.1989.tb00480.x. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
9. Donnellan MB, Conger RD, Bryant CM. The big five and enduring marriages. J. Res. Pers. 2004;38:481–504. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2004.01.001. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
10. Robins RW, Caspi A, Moffitt TE. It’s not just who you’re with, it’s who you are: Personality and relationship experiences across multiple relationships. J. Pers. 2002;70:925–964. doi:10.1111/1467-6494.05028. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
11. Kochanska G, Friesenborg AE, Lange LA, Martel MM. Parents’ personality and infants’ temperament as contributors to their emerging relationship. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2004;86:744–759. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.86.5.744. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
12. John OP, Srivastava S. The big five trait taxonomy: History, measurement, and theoretical perspectives. Hand. Pers.: Theory Res. 1999;2:102–138. [Google Scholar]
13. Schmitt DP, Allik J, McCrae RR, Benet-Martínez V. The geographic distribution of big five personality traits: Patterns and profiles of human self-description across 56 nations. J. Cross Cult. Psychol. 2007;38:173–212. doi:10.1177/0022022106297299. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
14. WHO (World Health Organization). (2001). Strengthening mental health promotion: Mental health is not just the absence of mental disorder.
15. Picco L, et al. Positive mental health among health professionals working at a psychiatric hospital. PLoS ONE. 2017;12:e0178359. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0178359. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
16. Goldberg, D. P. The detection of psychiatric illness by questionnaire. Maudsley Monograph, 21 (1972).
17. Donath S. The validity of the 12-item general health questionnaire in Australia: A comparison between three scoring methods. Aust. N. Z. J. Psychiatry. 2001;35:231–235. doi:10.1046/j.1440-1614.2001.00869.x. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
18. Schrnitz N, Kruse J, Tress W. Psychometric properties of the general health questionnaire (GHQ-12) in a German primary care sample. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 1999;100:462–468. doi:10.1111/j.1600-0447.1999.tb10898.x. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
19. Goldberg, D. P. User's guide to the general health questionnaire. (Windsor, 1988).
20. Jackson C. The general health questionnaire. Occup. Med. 2007;57:79–79. doi:10.1093/occmed/kql169. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
21. Goldberg DP, et al. The validity of two versions of the GHQ in the WHO study of mental illness in general health care. Psychol. Med. 1997;27:191–197. doi:10.1017/S0033291796004242. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
22. Montazeri A, et al. The 12-item general health questionnaire (GHQ-12): Translation and validation study of the Iranian version. Health Qual. Life Outcomes. 2003;1:1–4. doi:10.1186/1477-7525-1-1. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
23. del Pilar Sánchez-López M, Dresch V. The 12-item general health questionnaire (GHQ-12): Reliability, external validity and factor structure in the Spanish population. Psicothema. 2008;20:839–843. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
24. Campbell A, Knowles S. A confirmatory factor analysis of the GHQ12 using a large Australian sample. Eur. J. Psychol. Assess. 2007;23:2–8. doi:10.1027/1015-5759.23.1.2. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
25. Banks MH, et al. The use of the General health questionnaire as an indicator of mental health in occupational studies. J. Occup. Psychol. 1980;53:187–194. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8325.1980.tb00024.x. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
26. Graetz B. Multidimensional properties of the general health questionnaire. Soc. Psychiatry Psychiatr. Epidemiol. 1991;26:132–138. doi:10.1007/BF00782952. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
27. Padrón A, Galán I, Durbán M, Gandarillas A, Rodríguez-Artalejo F. Confirmatory factor analysis of the general health questionnaire (GHQ-12) in Spanish adolescents. Qual. Life Res. 2012;21:1291–1298. doi:10.1007/s11136-011-0038-x. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
28. Penninkilampi-Kerola V, Miettunen J, Ebeling H. A comparative assessment of the factor structures and psychometric properties of the GHQ-12 and the GHQ-20 based on data from a finnish population-based sample. Scand. J. Psychol. 2006;47:431–440. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9450.2006.00551.x. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
29. Krueger RF, Tackett JL. Personality and psychopathology: Working toward the bigger picture. J. Pers. Disord. 2003;17(2):109–128. doi:10.1521/pedi.17.2.109.23986. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
30. Nigg JT. Temperament and developmental psychopathology. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry. 2006;47(3–4):395–422. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01612.x. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
31. Tackett JL. Evaluating models of the personality–psychopathology relationship in children and adolescents. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 2006;26(5):584–599. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2006.04.003. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
32. Whittle S, Allen NB, Lubman DI, Yücel M. The neurobiological basis of temperament: Towards a better understanding of psychopathology. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2006;30(4):511–525. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2005.09.003. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
33. Cloninger CR, Svrakic NM, Svrakic DM. Role of personality self-organization in development of mental order and disorder. Dev. Psychopathol. 1997;9:881–906. doi:10.1017/S095457949700148X. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
34. Davydov DM, Stewart R, Ritchie K, Chaudieu I. Resilience and mental health. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 2010;30:479–495. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2010.03.003. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
35. Gestsdóttir S, Lerner RM. Intentional self-regulation and positive youth development in early adolescence: Findings from the 4-H Study of positive youth development. Dev. Psychopathol. 2007;43:508–521. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.43.2.508. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
36. Cloninger CR, Cloninger KM. Person-centered therapeutics. Int. J. Pers. Cent. Med. 2011;1:43. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
37. Josefsson K, et al. Associations of personality profiles with various aspects of well-being: A population-based study. J. Affect. Disord. 2011;133:265–273. doi:10.1016/j.jad.2011.03.023. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
38. Butkovic A, Brkovic I, Bratko D. Predicting well-being from personality in adolescents and older adults. J. Happiness Stud. 2012;13:455–467. doi:10.1007/s10902-011-9273-7. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
39. Hu Y, Stewart-Brown S, Twigg L, Weich S. Can the 12-item general health questionnaire be used to measure positive mental health? Psychol. Med. 2007;37:1005–1013. doi:10.1017/S0033291707009993. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
40. Huppert FA, Walters DE, Day N, Elliott BJ. The factor structure of the general health questionnaire (GHQ-30): A reliability study on 6317 community residents. Br. J. Psychiatry. 1989;155:178–185. doi:10.1192/bjp.155.2.178. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
41. Teufel-Shone NI, et al. The validation of the 12-item general health questionnaire among ethnic Indian women living in the United Kingdom. Am. J. Health Behav. 2005;29:413. doi:10.5993/AJHB.29.5.4. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
42. Albuquerque I, de Lima MP, Matos M, Figueiredo C. Personality and subjective well-being: What hides behind global analyses? Soc. Indic. Res. 2012;105:447–460. doi:10.1007/s11205-010-9780-7. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
43. Brooks SK, et al. The psychological impact of quarantine and how to reduce it: Rapid review of the evidence. Lancet. 2020;395:912–920. doi:10.1016/S0140-6736(20)30460-8. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
44. Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. “Normal” personality inventories in clinical assessment: General requirements and the potential for using the NEO personality inventory": Reply. 20 (1992).
45. Kotov R, Gamez W, Schmidt F, Watson D. Linking, “big” personality traits to anxiety, depressive, and substance use disorders: A meta-analysis. Psychol. Bull. 2010;136:768. doi:10.1037/a0020327. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
46. Lahey BB. Public health significance of neuroticism. Am. Psychol. 2009;64:241. doi:10.1037/a0015309. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
47. Otonari J, et al. Neuroticism and extraversion personality traits, health behaviours, and subjective well-being: The Fukuoka study (Japan) Qual. Life Res. Int. J. Qual. Life Asp. Treat Care Rehab. 2012;21:1847–1855. doi:10.1007/s11136-011-0098-y. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
48. Salama-Younes M, Montazeri A, Ismaïl A, Roncin C. Factor structure and internal consistency of the 12-item general health questionnaire (GHQ-12) and the subjective vitality scale (VS), and the relationship between them: A study from France. Health Qual. Life Outcomes. 2009;7:22. doi:10.1186/1477-7525-7-22. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
49. Mitchell LL, Zmora R, Finlay JM, Jutkowitz E, Gaugler JE. Do big five personality traits moderate the effects of stressful life events on health trajectories? Evidence from the health and retirement study. J. Geront. Ser. B. 2021;76:44–55. doi:10.1093/geronb/gbaa075. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
50. Diener E, Suh EM, Lucas RE, Smith HL. Subjective well-being: Three decades of progress. Psychol. Bull. 1999;125:276. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.125.2.276. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
51. Endsley P, Weobong B, Nadkarni A. The psychometric properties of GHQ for detecting common mental disorder among community dwelling men in Goa India. Asian J. Psychiatry. 2017;28:106–110. doi:10.1016/j.ajp.2017.03.023. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
52. Baksheev GN, Robinson J, Cosgrave EM, Baker K, Yung AR. Validity of the 12-item general health questionnaire (GHQ-12) in detecting depressive and anxiety disorders among high school students. Psychiatry Res. 2011;187:291–296. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2010.10.010. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
53. Doi Y, Minowa M. Factor structure of the 12-item general health questionnaire in the Japanese general adult population. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 2003;57:379–383. doi:10.1046/j.1440-1819.2003.01135.x. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
54. Lamers SM, Westerhof GJ, Kovács V, Bohlmeijer ET. Differential relationships in the association of the big five personality traits with positive mental health and psychopathology. J. Res. Pers. 2012;46:517–524. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2012.05.012. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
55. Smillie LD, Kern ML, Uljarevic M. Extroversion: Description, development, and mechanisms. In: McAdams DP, Shiner RL, Tackett JL, editors. Handbook of Personality development. London: Guilford Press; 2021. [Google Scholar]
56. Zelenski, J. M., Sobocko, K., & Whelan, A. D. C. Introversion, solitude, and subjective well‐being, In The Handbook of Solitude: Psychological Perspectives on Social Isolation, Social Withdrawal, and Being Alone, 184–201 (2013).
57. John, O. P., Naumann, L. P., & Soto, C. J. Paradigm shift to the integrative big five trait taxonomy: history, measurement, and conceptual issues. (2008).
58. McCabe KO, Fleeson W. What is extraversion for? integrating trait and motivational perspectives and identifying the purpose of extraversion. Psychol. Sci. 2012;23:1498–1505. doi:10.1177/0956797612444904. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
59. Watson D, Clark LA, McIntyre CW, Hamaker S. Affect, personality, and social activity. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1992;63:1011. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.63.6.1011. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
60. Goodwin R, Engstrom G. Personality and the perception of health in the general population. Psychol. Med. 2002;32:325–332. doi:10.1017/S0033291701005104. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
61. Anglim J, Horwood S, Smillie LD, Marrero RJ, Wood JK. Predicting psychological and subjective well-being from personality: A meta-analysis. Psychol. Bull. 2020;146:279. doi:10.1037/bul0000226. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
62. Lee RM, Dean BL, Jung KR. Social connectedness, extraversion, and subjective well-being: Testing a mediation model. Pers. Individ. Dif. 2008;45:414–419. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2008.05.017. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
63. Steel P, Schmidt J, Shultz J. Refining the relationship between personality and subjective well-being. Psychol. Bull. 2008;134:138. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.134.1.138. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
64. Brajša-Žganec A, Ivanović D, Kaliterna Lipovčan L. Personality traits and social desirability as predictors of subjective well-being. Psihol. Teme. 2011;20:261–276. [Google Scholar]
65. Smith AB, Fallowfield LJ, Stark DP, Velikova G, Jenkins V. A Rasch and confirmatory factor analysis of the general health questionnaire (GHQ)-12. Health Qual. Life Outcomes. 2010;8:1–10. doi:10.1186/1477-7525-8-45. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
66. Daradkeh TK, Ghubash R, El-Rufaie OE. Reliability, validity, and factor structure of the Arabic version of the 12-item general health questionnaire. Psychol. Rep. 2001;89:85–94. doi:10.2466/pr0.2001.89.1.85. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
67. Watson, D., & Clark, L. A. Extraversion and its positive emotional core, In Handbook of Personality Psychology 767–793 (Academic Press, 1997).
68. Hakulinen C, Elovainio M, Pulkki-Råback L, Virtanen M, Kivimäki M, Jokela M. Personality and depressive symptoms: Individual participant meta-analysis of 10 cohort studies. Depress. Anxiety. 2015;32(7):461–470. doi:10.1002/da.22376. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
69. Gray JS, Pinchot JJ. Predicting health from self and partner personality. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2018;121:48–51. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2017.09.019. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
70. Lewis EG, Cardwell JM. The big five personality traits, perfectionism and their association with mental health among UK students on professional degree programmes. BMC Psychol. 2020;8(1):1–10. doi:10.1186/s40359-020-00423-3. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
71. University of Essex Institute for social and economic research. Br. Househ. Panel Surv. Waves. 2018;1–18:1991–2009. [Google Scholar]
72. Donnellan MB, Lucas RE. Age differences in the big five across the life span: Evidence from two national samples. Psychol. Aging. 2008;23:558–566. doi:10.1037/a0012897. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
73. Gerlitz, J. Y., & Schupp, J. Zur Erhebung der Big-Five-basierten persoenlichkeitsmerkmale im SOEP. DIW Res. Notes.4, (2005).
74. Lang FR, John D, Ludtke O, Schupp J, Wagner GG. Short assessment of the Big Five: Robust accros survey methods except telephone interviewing. Behav. Res. Methods. 2011;43:548–567. doi:10.3758/s13428-011-0066-z. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
75. Tavakol M, Dennick R. Making sense of Cronbach's alpha. Int. J. Med. Ed. 2011;2:53–55. doi:10.5116/ijme.4dfb.8dfd. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
76. Fergusson DM, Horwood LJ, Lawton JM. The relationships between neuroticism and depressive symptoms. Soc. Psychiatry Psychiatr. Epidemiol. 1989;24:275–281. doi:10.1007/BF01788029. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
77. Schmutte PS, Ryff CD. Personality and well-being: Reexamining methods and meanings. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1997;73(3):549. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.73.3.549. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
78. Spijker J, de Graaf R, Oldehinkel AJ, Nolen WA, Ormel J. Are the vulnerability effects of personality and psychosocial functioning on depression accounted for by subthreshold symptoms? Depress. Anxiety. 2007;24(7):472–478. doi:10.1002/da.20252. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
79. Wolfenstein M, Trull TJ. Depression and openness to experience. J. Pers. Assess. 1997;69:614–632. doi:10.1207/s15327752jpa6903_14. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
80. Karsten J, et al. The state effect of depressive and anxiety disorders on big five personality traits. J. Psychiatr. Res. 2012;46:644–650. doi:10.1016/j.jpsychires.2012.01.024. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
81. Yu Y, Zhao Y, Li D, Zhang J, Li J. The relationship between big five personality and social well-being of Chinese residents: The mediating effect of social support. Front. Psychol. 2020;11:613659. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2020.613659. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
82. Kaplan SC, Levinson CA, Rodebaugh TL, Menatti A, Weeks JW. Social anxiety and the big five personality traits: The interactive relationship of trust and openness. Cogn. Behav. Ther. 2015;44(3):212–222. doi:10.1080/16506073.2015.1008032. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
83. Burns KM, Burns NR, Ward L. Confidence—More a personality or ability trait? It depends on how it is measured: A comparison of young and older adults. Front. Psychol. 2016;7:518. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00518. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
84. Sukenik S, Reizer A, Koslovsky M. Direct and indirect effects of Agreeableness on overconfidence. J. Ind. Dif. 2018 doi:10.1027/1614-0001/a000261. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
85. Friedman HS. The multiple linkages of personality and disease. Brain Behav. Immun. 2008;22:668–675. doi:10.1016/j.bbi.2007.09.004. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
86. Hampson SE. Mechanisms by which childhood personality traits influence adult well-being. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2008;17(4):264–268. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8721.2008.00587.x. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
87. Hopwood CJ, Newman DA, Donnellan MB, Markowitz JC, Grilo CM, Sanislow CA, Ansell EB, McGlashan TH, Skodol AE, Shea MT, Gunderson JG, Zanarini MC, Morey LC. The stability of personality traits in individuals with borderline personality disorder. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 2009;118(4):806–815. doi:10.1037/a0016954. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
88. Friedman HS, Kern ML, Hampson SE, Duckworth AL. A new life-span approach to conscientiousness and health: Combining the pieces of the causal puzzle. Dev. Psychol. 2014;50(5):1377–1389. doi:10.1037/a0030373. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
89. Schaefer PS, Williams CC, Goodie AS, Campbell WK. Overconfidence and the big five. J Res Pers. 2004;38:473–480. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2003.09.010. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
90. Kokko K, Tolvanen A, Pulkkinen L. Associations between personality traits and psychological well-being across time in middle adulthood. J. Res. Pers. 2013;47:748–756. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2013.07.002. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
91. Watson D, Stasik SM, Ellickson-Larew S, Stanton K. Extraversion and psychopathology: A facet-level analysis. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 2015;124(2):432–446. doi:10.1037/abn0000051. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
92. Stanton K, Watson D. Positive and negative affective dysfunction in psychopathology. Soc. Pers. Psych. Comp. 2014;8(9):555–567. doi:10.1111/spc3.12132. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
93. Watson D, Stanton K, Khoo S, Ellickson-Larew S, Stasik-O'Brien SM. Extraversion and psychopathology: A multilevel hierarchical review. J. Res. Pers. 2019;81:1–10. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2019.04.009. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
94. Hahn E, Gottschling J, Spinath FM. Short measurements of personality–validity and reliability of the GSOEP big five inventory (BFI-S) J. Res. Pers. 2012;46(3):355–359. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2012.03.008. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
95. Lang FR, John D, Lüdtke O, Schupp J, Wagner GG. Short assessment of the big five: Robust across survey methods except telephone interviewing. Behav. Res. Meth. 2011;43(2):548–567. doi:10.3758/s13428-011-0066-z. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
96. Soto CJ, John OP. The next big five inventory (BFI-2): Developing and assessing a hierarchical model with 15 facets to enhance bandwidth, fidelity, and predictive power. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2017;113(1):117–143. doi:10.1037/pspp0000096. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
97. Gosling SD, Rentfrow PJ, Swann WB., Jr A very brief measure of the Big-Five personality domains. J. Res. Pers. 2003;37(6):504–528. doi:10.1016/S0092-6566(03)00046-1. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
Articles from Scientific Reports are provided here courtesy of Nature Publishing Group